´ëÇѾð¾îÇÐȸ ÀüÀÚÀú³Î

´ëÇѾð¾îÇÐȸ

Table of Contents

24±Ç 4È£ (2016³â 12¿ù)

À½¿î ÀÌ¿ô´Ü¾î¿Í Çѱ¹¾î ¾îµÎ ¾ç¼ø ¹× Ä¡°æ Æä¼âÀ½ÀÇ À½¼ºÀû ½ÇÇö

ÀÌ¿ëÀº

Pages : 197-217

DOI :

PDFº¸±â

¸®½ºÆ®

Abstract

Lee, Yongeun. (2016). Phonological neighbors and phonetic realizations of Korean word-initial bilabial and alveolar stops. The Linguistic Association of Korea Journal, 24(4), 197-217. In English, it has been shown that mean VOT for word initial voiceless stops is statistically longer when they occur in a word with a minimal-pair neighbor (e.g., cod vs. god) than in a word without such a neighbor (cop vs. *gop) (Baese-Berke & Goldrick, 2009; Buz, Tanenhaus, & Jaeger, 2016). The present study investigated whether such lexical effect of phonological neighbors can be extended to phonetic realizations of word-initial Korean stops. Measurements of VOT of the plain and aspirated Korean bilabial and alveolar stops (a total of 2640 tokens elicited from eleven speakers) along with F0 and H1-H2 of their following vowels suggested no evidence of the role of phonological neighbors in the realizations of the stops. The present results suggest that unlike English the role of phonological neighbors in the realizations of Korean stops may be limited at least in tasks where speakers are asked to simply read aloud words containing the target stops. Discussions of the present results along with a suggestion for a future study, particularly exploring the role of listeners in the production of Korean stops, are provided.

Keywords

# À½¿îÀÌ¿ô # ÃּҴ븳½Ö # Çѱ¹¾î Æó¼âÀ½ # VOT # F0 # H1-H2.

References

  • °­¹ü¸ð, ±èÈï±Ô. (2004). Çѱ¹¾î ÇüÅÂ¼Ò ¹× ¾îÈÖ »ç¿ë ºóµµÀÇ ºÐ¼® 2: 550¸¸ ¾îÀýÀÇ ¼¼Á¾ ÇüÅÂÀǹ̺м® ¸»¹¶Ä¡¸¦ ±â¹ÝÀ¸·Î. ÄÄÇ»ÅÍ¿Í Àι®ÇÐ ½Ã¸®Áî 10. ¼­¿ï: °í·Á´ëÇб³ ¹ÎÁ·¹®È­¿¬±¸¿ø.
  • ±ÇÀ¯¾È, Á¶Çý¼÷, ±èÃæ¸í, ³²±âÃá. (2006). Çѱ¹¾î ½Ã°¢´Ü¾îÀçÀο¡¼­ ³ªÅ¸³ª´Â ÀÌ¿ôÈ¿°ú. ¸»¼Ò¸®, 60, 29-45.
  • ±ÇÀ¯¾È, ³²±âÃá. (2011). ÇÑ±Û À½Àý ÀÌ¿ô È¿°ú¿¡¼­ ÇÑÀÚ¾î ÇüżÒÀÇ ¿µÇâ: Ç¥±â ¹× À½¿î ÀÌ¿ô°ú ÇÑÀÚ¾î ÀÌ¿ô°úÀÇ °ü·Ã¼ºÀ» Áß½ÉÀ¸·Î. Çѱ¹½É¸®ÇÐȸÁö:ÀÎÁö ¹× »ý¹°, 23, 301-319.
  • ±è¹Ì·É. (2015). Çѱ¹¾î-¿µ¾î ÀÌÁß ¾ð¾î È­ÀÚµéÀÇ L1°ú L2 ¿µÇâ¿¡ °üÇÑ ¿¬±¸: VOT¿Í F0 °ü·ÃÇؼ­. ¸»¼Ò¸®¿Í À½¼º°úÇÐ, 7, 13-26.
  • Baese-Berke, M., & Goldrick, M. (2009). Mechanisms of interaction in speech production. Language and Cognitive Process, 24, 527-554.
  • Bang, H., Sonderegger, M., Kang, Y., Clayardsa, M., & Yoon. T. (2015). The effect of word frequency on the timecourse of tonogenesis in Seoul Korean. Proceedings of the International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Glasgow, UK.
  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker and Walker, S. (2015). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 1.1-9. Available at http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4 (Last viewed October 2, 2015).
  • Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2014). Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer program], version 5.4.12. Available at http://www.praat.org (Last viewed July 10, 2015).
  • Buz, E., Tanenhaus, M., & Jaeger, F. (2016). Dynamically adapted context-specific hyper-articulation: Feedback from interlocutors affects speakers¡¯ subsequent pronunciations. Journal of Memory and Language, 89, 68-86.
  • Choi, Y., & Choi, M. (2016). Infant¡¯s speech perception research: A literature review and an outlook for related Korean studies. Communication Sciences and Disorders, 21, 1-14.
  • Cho, T., Jun, S.-A,, & Ladefoged, P. (2002). Acoustic and aerodynamic correlates of Korean stops and fricatives. Journal of Phonetics, 30, 193-228.
  • Cho, T., Lee, Y., & Kim, S. (2011). Communicatively driven versus prosodically driven hyper-articulation in Korean. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 344-361.
  • Dell, G. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283-321.
  • Goldrick, M. (2006). Limited interaction in speech production: Chronometric, speech error and neuropsychological evidence. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 817-855.
  • Goldinger, S., & Summers W. (1989). Primining lexical neighbors of spoken words: Effects of competition and inhibition. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 501-518.
  • Kang, K.-H., & Guion, S. (2008). Clear speech production of Korean stops: Changing phonetic targets and enhancement strategies. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124, 3909-3917.
  • Kang, Y. (2014). Voice onset time merger and development of tonal contrast in Seoul Korean stops: a corpus study. Journal of Phonetics, 45, 76-90.
  • Kim, M.-R., Beddor, P., & Horrocks, J. (2002). The contribution of consonantal and vocalic information to the perception of Korean initial stops. Journal of Phonetics, 30, 77-100.
  • Kim, M.-R. (2014). Ongoing sound change in the stop system of Korean: A three- to two-way categorization. Studies in Phonetics, Phonology, and Morphology, 20, 51-82.
  • Kirov, C., & Wilson, C. (2012). The specificity of online variation in speech production. In Proceedings of the 34th annual conference of the cognitive science society, 587-592.
  • Kong, E., & Lee, H. (2016). Attentional modulation and individual differences in explaining the changing role of F0 in the Korean laryngeal stop perception. Paper presented at the 15th conference on laboratory phonology, Itacha, NY, 13-17 July.
  • Oh, E. (2011). Effects of speaker gender on voice onset time in Korean stops. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 59-67.
  • Oh, M., & Yang, H. (2013). The production of stops by Seoul and Yanbian Korean speakers. Journal of the Korean Society of Speech Sciences, 5, 185-193.
  • R Development Core Team. (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Available at https://www.R-project.org/ (Last viewed October 2, 2015).
  • Pate, J., & Goldwater, S. (2015). Talkers account for listener and channel characteristics to communicate efficiently. Journal of Memory and Language, 78, 1-17.
  • Schertz, J. (2013). Exaggeration of featural contrasts in clarifications of misheard speech in English. Journal of Phonetics, 41, 249-263.
  • Silva, D. (2006). Acoustic evidence for the emergence of tonal contrast in contemporary Korean. Phonology, 23, 287-308.
  • Uther, M., Knoll, M., & Burnham, D. (2007). Do you speak E-NG-L-I-SH? A comparison of foreigner- and infant-directed speech. Speech Communication, 49, 2-7.